Cardboxes in unstructured motor activity with non-walkers in a nursery: exploratory study
PDF (Português)

Keywords

Loose parts
unstructured activities
play
dynamic ecological pedagogy
nursery

How to Cite

Serrão-Arrais, A., Catela, D., & Luís, H. (2024). Cardboxes in unstructured motor activity with non-walkers in a nursery: exploratory study. Encontro Nacional De Educação Básica. https://doi.org/10.34630/eneb.vi.5975

Abstract

In unstructured activities, children freely explore self-regulated behaviors. The loose parts model proposes non-functional mobile materials in children's spaces, without adult interference; with pre-school studies revealing it potential; however, we did not find studies in nursery. Eight babies (11.5±1.32 months old, 7 non-walkers) had free access, in the nursery, for 18 minutes, to 12 cardboard boxes of different colors with prehensible dimensions, 6 with content to produce sound, and 1 large movable and scaled box. Babies spent ≈70% of their time standing still or moving without contact with boxes; but, all interacted with them, with an average frequency of 9.88(±4.78) different behaviors per child. Three children established a sequential interaction, in pairs, around the exploration of a small and a large box. Thus, the cardboard boxes allowed babies to engage in autonomous exploratory and physical play, through fine and gross motor skills, revealing some positive and negative non-verbal interaction; also providing episodes of parallel play. Apparently, passive behaviors may have played an important role in their free play. Therefore, there is motor and playful potential in the loose parts model with babies, in an unstructured large group activity, with just one type of object. The pedagogical strategy implemented, a combination of unstructured activity and loose materials, is an example of dynamic ecological pedagogy, with the expression of expected phenomena, such as non-proportionality and multistability, both for motor and playful behaviors, through the regulation of the dimension of card boxes.

https://doi.org/10.34630/eneb.vi.5975
PDF (Português)

References

Babik, I., Galloway, J. C., & Lobo, M. A. (2022). Early exploration of one’s own body, exploration of objects, and motor, language, and cognitive development relate dynamically across the first two years of life. Developmental psychology, 58(2), 222-235. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001289

Brockman, R., Jago, R., & Fox, K. R. (2011). Children's active play: self-reported motivators, barriers and facilitators. BMC public health, 11, 461. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-11-461

Brussoni, M., Olsen, L. L., Pike, I., & Sleet, D. A. (2012). Risky play and children’s safety: Balancing priorities for optimal child development. International journal of environmental research and public health, 9(9), 3134-3148. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph9093134

Bundy, A. C., Naughton, G., Tranter, P., Wyver, S., Baur, L., Schiller, W., ... & Brentnall, J. (2011). The Sydney playground project: popping the bubblewrap-unleashing the power of play: a cluster randomized controlled trial of a primary school playground-based intervention aiming to increase children's physical activity and social skills. BMC public health, 11, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-11-680

Carta, J. J., Greenwood, C. R., Luze, G. J., Cline, G., & Kuntz, S. (2004). Developing a General Outcome Measure of Growth in Social Skills for Infants and Toddlers. Journal of Early Intervention, 26(2), 91–114. https://doi.org/10.1177/105381510402600203

Chemero, A. (2003). An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological psychology, 15(2), 181-195. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326969ECO1502_5

Chien, N. C., Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Pianta, R. C., Ritchie, S., Bryant, D. M., ... & Barbarin, O. A. (2010). Children’s classroom engagement and school readiness gains in prekindergarten. Child development, 81(5), 1534-1549. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01490.x

Clark, J. E., & Metcalfe, J. S. (2002). The mountain of motor development: A metaphor. Motor development: Research and reviews, 2(163-190), 183-202.

Clearfield, M. W., Osborne, C. N., & Mullen, M. (2008). Learning by looking: Infants’ social looking behavior across the transition from crawling to walking. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100(4), 297–307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.03.005

Fisher, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K .,Golinkoff, R. M., Singer, D., & Berk, L. E. (2011). Playing around in school: Implications for learning and educational policy. In A. Pellegrini (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of play (pp. 341-363). NY: Oxford University Press. ISBN: 9780129539300

Gibson, J. J. (2014). The ecological approach to visual perception: classic edition. Psychology press.

Gibson, J. L., Cornell, M., & Gill, T. (2017). A systematic review of research into the impact of loose parts play on children’s cognitive, social and emotional development. School mental health, 9(4), 295-309. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-017-9220-9

Green, C. (2013). A Sense of Autonomy in Young Children's Special Places. International Journal of Early Childhood Environmental Education, 1(1), 8-31. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ej1108047

Gull, C., Bogunovich, J., Goldstein, S. L., & Rosengarten, T. (2019). Definitions of Loose Parts in Early Childhood Outdoor Classrooms: A Scoping Review. International Journal of Early Childhood Environmental Education, 6(3), 37–52. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1225658

Hanish, L. D., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Spinrad, T. L., Ryan, P., & Schmidt, S. (2004). The expression and regulation of negative emotions: Risk factors for young children's peer victimization. Development and psychopathology, 16(2), 335-353. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579404044542

Harman, B., & Harms, C. (2017). Predictors of unstructured play amongst preschool children in Australia. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 42(3), 31-37. https://doi.org/10.23965/AJEC.42.3.04

Hofsten, C. von. (2012). Afterword: Infants’ explorative actions. An important aspect of early cognitive development. Enfance, N° 1(1), 115–121. https://doi.org/10.3917/enf1.121.0115

Holloway, S. D., & Reichhart-Erickson, M. (1988). The relationship of day care quality to children’s free-play behavior and social problem-solving skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 3(1), 39–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2006(88)90028-2

Holt, N. L., Lee, H., Millar, C. A., & Spence, J. C. (2015). ‘Eyes on where children play’: A retrospective study of active free play. Children's Geographies, 13(1), 73-88. https://doi.org/10.1080/14733285.2013.828449

Howes, C. (1980). Peer play scale as an index of complexity of peer interaction. Developmental Psychology, 16(4), 371–372. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.16.4.371

Jennings, K. D. (1975). People versus object orientation, social behavior, and intellectual abilities in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 11(4), 511–519. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076679

Kamm, K., Thelen, E., & Jensen, J. L. (1990). A dynamical systems approach to motor development. Physical therapy, 70(12), 763-775. https://doi.org/10.1093/ptj/70.12.763

Katz, J. C., & Buchholz, E. S. (1999). “I did it myself”: The necessity of solo play for preschoolers. Early Child Development and Care, 155(1), 39-50. https://doi.org/10.1080/0030443991550104

Kelso, J. S. (2012). Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic coordination in the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1591), 906-918. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0351

Kestenbaum, R., Termine, N., & Spelke, E. S. (1987). Perception of objects and object boundaries by 3‐month‐old infants. British journal of developmental psychology, 5(4), 367-383.

Kuh, l. P., Ponte, i., & Chau, C. (2013). The Impact of a Natural Playscape Installation on Young Children’s Play Behaviors. Children, Youth and Environments, 23(2), 49. https://doi.org/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.23.2.0049

Lavrysen, A., Bertrands, E., Leyssen, L., Smets, L., Vanderspikken, A., & De Graef, P. (2017). Risky-play at school. Facilitating risk perception and competence in young children. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 25(1), 89-105. https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2015.1102412

Lee, R. L. T., Lane, S., Brown, G., Leung, C., Kwok, S. W. H., & Chan, S. W. C. (2020). Systematic review of the impact of unstructured play interventions to improve young children's physical, social, and emotional wellbeing. Nursing & Health Sciences, 22(2), 184-196. https://doi.org/10.1111/nhs.12732

Lemay, L., Bigras, N., & Bouchard, C. (2016). Respecting but not sustaining play: early childhood educators’ and home childcare providers’ practices that support children’s play. Early years, 36(4), 383-398. https://doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2016.1149453

Linn, M. I., Goodman, J. F., & Lender, W. L. (2000). Played Out? Passive Behavior by Children With Down Syndrome During Unstructured Play. Journal of Early Intervention, 23(4), 264–278. https://doi.org/10.1177/10538151000230040601

Mathieson, K., & Banerjee, R. (2010). Pre-school peer play: The beginnings of social competence. Educational and child Psychology, 27(1), 9-20.

Maxwell, L. E., Mitchell, M. R., & Evans, G. W. (2008). Effects of play equipment and loose parts on preschool children’s outdoor play behavior: An observational study and design intervention. Children Youth and Environments, 18(2), 36–63. https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.18.2.0036

Mercê, C., Davids, K., Catela, D., Branco, M., Correia, V., & Cordovil, R. (2023). Learning to cycle: a constraint-led intervention programme using different cycling task constraints. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1080/17408989.2023.2185599

Muentener, P., Herrig, E., & Schulz, L. (2018). The efficiency of infants' exploratory play is related to longer-term cognitive development. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 635. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00635

Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. In M.G. Wade, H. T. A. Whiting (Eds.), Motor development in children: aspects of coordination and control (pp. 341-361). Amsterdam: Martin Nijhoff.

Newell, K. M., Liu, Y. T., & Mayer-Kress, G. (2003). A dynamical systems interpretation of epigenetic landscapes for infant motor development. Infant Behavior and Development, 26(4), 449-472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.08.003

Nicholson, S. (1972). The theory of loose parts, an important principle for design methodology. Studies in Design Education Craft & Technology, 4(2), 5–14. http://jil.lboro.ac.uk/ojs/index.php/SDEC/article/view/1204

Nicolopoulou, A. (2010). The alarming disappearance of play from early childhood education. Human development, 53(1), 1-4. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26764938

Pellis, S. M., & Pellis, V. C. (2007). Rough-and-Tumble Play and the Development of the Social Brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 95–98. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00483.x

Sidhu, J., Barlas, N., & Lifter, K. (2022). On the meanings of functional play: a review and clarification of definitions. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 42(2), 189-201. https://doi.org/10.1177/02711214209518

Sluss, D. (2005). Supporting play: Birth to age eight. Clifton Park, NY: Thomson Delmar Learning.

Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cognitive science, 14(1), 29-56. https://doi.org/10.1016/0364-0213(90)90025-R

Thiessen, M., Gluth, S., & Corso, R. (2013). Unstructured play and creative development in the classroom. International Journal for Cross-Disciplinary Subjects in Education (IJCDSE), 4(4), 1341-1348.

Vereijken, B. (2010). The complexity of childhood development: variability in perspective. Physical therapy, 90(12), 1850-1859. https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20100019

von Hofsten, C., & Spelke, E. S. (1985). Object perception and object-directed reaching in infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114(2), 198-212.

Wood, E. (2007). New directions in play: consensus or collision?. Education 3–13, 35(4), 309-320. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004270701602426

Creative Commons License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.