Desenho próprio de espaço reduzido conhecido para atividade de orientação em crianças de 4-5 anos de idade
PDF (Português)

Keywords

Orienteering activity
graphic development stage
own drawing
interdisciplinarity
preschool

How to Cite

Catela, D., Serrão-Arrais, A., Brito, C., Cerejeira, L., Costa, B., Graça, F., & Pereira, A. (2024). Desenho próprio de espaço reduzido conhecido para atividade de orientação em crianças de 4-5 anos de idade. Encontro Nacional De Educação Básica. https://doi.org/10.34630/eneb.vi.5965

Abstract

Children are able to draw spatial organizations and use aerial photographs or their own drawings to orient themselves in a location. With the present study we intend to analyze whether the child's graphic development stage would affect their ability in an orientation activity. The sample consisted of 38 children (girls= 24) aged 4.58(±0.5), of which 16 were 4 years old; with informed consent and assent. The children used a drawing they made of their classroom, to carry out an orienteering activity. Graphic development stage was based on qualitative analysis, and the orienteering activity on quantitative data. Children without graphic development of intellectual realism did the orienteering activity with an aerial photography. The results revealed that 4-year-old children who invested more in graphic details had a greater capacity for autonomy in the orienteering activity. Thus, at 4 years of age, the child's graphic development may condition autonomy in the orienteering activity, but not impede it. The graphic representation of a location and the practice of orienteering are complementary didactic activities, with combined fulfillment of objectives defined in the Curricular Guidelines for Pre-School Education. The data collected provides useful information for the implementation of a composite interdisciplinary activity, of a multidisciplinary type, with the central elements being the graphic representation of a known place, used as a personal map for an orienteering activity in that place.

https://doi.org/10.34630/eneb.vi.5965
PDF (Português)

References

Allen, G. L., & Ondracek, P. J. (1995). Age-sensitive cognitive abilities related to children’s acquisition of spatial knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 934–945. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.6.934

Barroso, M. (2014). Orientação com crianças dos 3 aos 5 anos de idade, com fotografia aérea oblíqua num espaço ao ar livre: influência da familiaridade com o local e com a orientação desportiva. Universidade da Madeira. https://digituma.uma.pt/bitstream/10400.13/1466/3/DoutoramentoMarisaBarroso.pdf

Barroso, M., Bento, T., & Catela, D. (2014). A orientação em crianças dos 3 aos 5 anos-muito mais que só a idade. In C. Neto, J. Barreiros, R. Cordovil, & F. Melo (Eds.), Estudos em Desenvolvimento Motor da Criança VIII (pp. 279–285). FMH.

Bjørgen, K. (2016). Physical activity in light of affordances in outdoor environments: qualitative observation studies of 3–5 years olds in kindergarten. Springerplus, 5(1), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40064-016-2565-y

Blaut, J. M., Stea, D., Spencer, C., & Blades, M. (2003). Mapping as a Cultural and Cognitive Universal. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 93(1), 165–185. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8306.93111

Bluestein, N., & Acredolo, L. (1979). Developmental Changes in Map-Reading Skills. Child Development, 50(3), 691. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128934

Carreira, B., Rosado, B., Ferreira, C., Marques, C., Watché, D., Domingues, F., ... & Catela, D. (2019). Estudo piloto sobre orientação no pré-escolar com fotografia e com desenho colorido próprio na respetiva sala. In K. O’Hara, B. Travassos &, C. Lourenço (Eds.), Estudos em Desenvolvimento Motor da Criança XIV (pp. 379-380). Covilhã, Portugal: UBI Edições. ISBN: 978-989-654-605-2.

Chettiparamb, A. (2011). Inter-disciplinarity in teaching: Probing urban studies. Journal for Education in the Built Environment, 6(1), 68-90. https://doi.org/10.11120/jebe.2011.06010068

DeLoache, J. S. (1991). Symbolic Functioning in Very Young Children: Understanding of Pictures and Models. Child Development, 62(4), 736–752. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01566.x

Fox, J. E., & Lee, J. (2013). When children draw vs when children don’t: exploring the effects of observational drawing in science. Creative Education, 4(7), 11-14. 10.4236/ce.2013.47A1002

Frick, A., Ferrara, K., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). Using a touch screen paradigm to assess the development of mental rotation between 3 1⁄2 and 5 1⁄2 years of age. Cognitive Processes, 14, 117–127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0534-0

Grady, R. (2022). Making Maps as Creating and Constructing Experiences in a Prekindergarten Forest Classroom. International Journal of Early Childhood Environmental Education, 9(2), 33-55. https://doi.org/10.1080/23748834.2019.1604931

Heft, H. (2013). Wayfinding, navigation, and environmental cognition from a naturalist’s stance. In D. Waller & L. Nadel (Eds.), Handbook of spatial cognition. (pp. 265–294). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13936-015

Huutoniemi, K., Klein, J. T., Bruun, H., & Hukkinen, J. (2010). Analyzing interdisciplinarity: Typology and indicators. Research policy, 39(1), 79-88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.respol.2009.09.011

Jansen-Osmann, P., & Wiedenbauer, G. (2004). The representation of landmarks and routes in children and adults: A study in a virtual environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 347–357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.08.003

Karakitsou, C. (1999). Assessment of cognitive development in four to eight year old children by means of drawing tasks. University of Stirling.

Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual-spatial working memory, and preschool quantity-number competencies on mathematics achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 516–531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.009

Lambert, E. B. (2006). Can Drawing Facilitate Problem-Solving? An Exploratory Study. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 31(2), 42–47. https://doi.org/10.1177/183693910603100207

Lange-Küttner, C. (2014). Do drawing stages really exist? Children’s early mapping of perspective. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(2), 168–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036199

Lerstrup, I., & Konijnendijk van den Bosch, C. (2017). Affordances of outdoor settings for children in preschool: Revisiting heft’s functional taxonomy. Landscape research, 42(1), 47-62. https://doi.org/10.1080/01426397.2016.1252039

Liben, L. S. (2002). Spatial development in childhood: Where are we now?. Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development, 326-348. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470996652.ch15

Luquet, G.-H. (1969). O desenho infantil. Ed. Minho.

Morra, S., Angi, A., & Tomat, L. (1996). Planning, Encoding, and Overcoming Conflict in Partial Occlusion Drawing: A Neo-Piagetian Model and an Experimental Analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61(3), 276–301. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0017

Navarro-Patón, R., Lago-Ballesteros, J., Arufe-Giráldez, V., Sanmiguel-Rodríguez, A., Lago-Fuentes, C., & Mecías-Calvo, M. (2021). Gender differences on motor competence in 5-year-old preschool children regarding relative age. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(6), 3143. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063143

Pereira, A., Costa, B., Vieira, B., Graça, F., Graça, F., Cerejeira, L., Brito, M. C., Serrão-Arrais, A., & Catela, D. (2022). Desenho próprio de espaço reduzido conhecido e atividade de orientação em crianças de 4-5 anos estudo exploratório [Poster] apresentado no XVII Seminário de Desenvolvimento Motor da Criança, Escola Superior de Educação e Comunicação-Universidade do Algarve, Faro, Portugal, 28-29 de outubro de 2022.

Presson, C. C., & Hazelrigg, M. D. (1984). Building spatial representations through primary and secondary learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(4), 716–722. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.716

Quillin, K., & Thomas, S. (2015). Drawing-to-learn: A framework for using drawings to promote model-based reasoning in biology. CBE - Life Sciences Education, 14(1), es2. https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe.14-08-0128

Reis, R., Martins, A., Silva, C., Sofia, L., Cruz, L., Martinho, R., Barroso, M., & Catela, D. (2018). Capacidade de orientação em espaço alargado não habitual no jardim de infância. In P. Rodrigues, A. Rebolo, F. Vieira, A. Dias, & L. Silva (Coord.), Estudos em Desenvolvimento Motor da Criança XIII (pp. 165-170). Edições Piaget. Lisboa, Portugal. ISBN: 978-898-121-1.

Rohde, J. F., Larsen, S. C., Sederberg, M., Bahrenscheer, A., Nielsen, A. K., Heitmann, B. L., & Specht, I. O. (2023). Outdoor Kindergartens: A Structural Way to Improve Early Physical Activity Behaviour?. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(6), 5131. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20065131

Silva, I. L., Marques, L., Mata, L., & Rosa, M. (2016). Orientações Curriculares para a Educação Pré-Escolar. Ministério da Educação/Direção-Geral da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-404-7. https://www.dge.mec.pt/ocepe/sites/default/files/Orientacoes_Curriculares.pdf

Uttal, D. H. (2000). Seeing the big picture: Map use and the development of spatial cognition. Developmental Science, 3(3), 247-264. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00119

Creative Commons License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.